RACIAL
RESIDENTIAL SEGREGATION AND RACIAL MEANING IN SOUTH SHORE
M.A.
Thesis Paper of Andrew Lawrence Crown
Written
while Mr. Crown was a student in The Department of Political Science
The
University of Chicago
Degree
awarded December, 1993
INTRODUCTION
Meaning
is something signified by a word or something one wishes to convey,
especially by language.[1]
People convey meaning through language, but language is not to be
conceived as something which conveys meaning in itself.
Language is a highly developed system of symbols.
"Meaning is not an inherent property of symbols; it is a result of
their use in particular contexts."[2]
When people talk about race relations they convey, through language,
meaning or meanings which I will refer to as racial meaning or racial
meanings. Meanings are in society and therefore in people.[3]
Racial meanings reflect what is taking place in society and in the
"real" or external and material world of race relations.
However, the racial meanings that people convey through language are not
mere reflections of race relations. These
meanings are also involved in the perpetuation of the economic inequality and
discrimination which have characterized American race relations.
Using
face to face interviews with employers, social scientists have studied how
employers use the meaning of "Black inner city underclass," a concept
representing the intersection between racial, class, and spatial categories, to
screen out Black inner city job applicants in an attempt to distinguish good
from bad workers.[4]
In Canarsie, New York, an increase in verbalized and other outward
displays of racism and prejudice among Jews with a history of urbanism and
liberalism both reflected and resulted in changes in sociological, economic, and
political contexts.[5]
The changes in the very labels people use to talk about race reflect
broader societal changes and contests over meaning.
The use of the term "African-American" initiated by Black
leaders has made inroads among the general Black population, but Whites have
been more likely than Blacks to favor the established term "Black,"
over "African-American." Each
change in racial labels, from Colored to Negro to Black to African-American, can
be seen as an attempt by Blacks to gain respect and standing through
self-definition.[6]
Often,
to talk about Black-White race relations in America is to talk about politics.
This is apparent whenever we casually observe people discussing some
aspect of American race relations as if it were a controversial political issue.
Such observations should not surprise us.
American race relations have long involved the struggle of Black
Americans to gain political rights in order to protect themselves from arbitrary
violence.[7]
Race relations in the United States are political, even according to a
narrow definition of politics as, striving to share power or striving to
influence the distribution of power, either among states or among groups within
a state, where a state is the human community that successfully claims a
monopoly over the legitimate use of physical force.[8]
The political nature of America's race problem arises from the fact that
Blacks were, from the very beginning of American Democracy, excluded from the
political system. In a nation which
aspires to great ideals through its political system, the exclusion of Blacks
from that system has led to recurrent spiritual and moral crises.
"Mental contradiction and moral contradiction have been the price
which the United States has paid again and again for refusing to face the
problem of its Negro population."[9]
In
order to study the racial meanings conveyed when people talk about race as if it
were a political issue, one could analyze political speeches or media messages
which explicitly and implicitly convey the racial ideologies that the elite
communicate to the masses through language.
Language is sign and symbol, and if the study of politics is the study of
influence and the influential, then symbol is the method of the influential
elite, and propaganda is the political language par excellence.
"By the use of sanctioned words and gestures the elite elicits
blood, work, taxes, applause, from the masses."[10]
If the science of politics is the science of power, then the language of
politics is the language of power and decision.
"It is battle cry, verdict and sentence, statute, ordinance and
rule, oath of office, controversial news, comment and debate."[11]
The
language of politics is often best conceptualized as something decisively
disseminated from "above." However,
when people talk about politics, race and politics included, they do not always
simply passively receive and regurgitate the symbols and messages disseminated
by the elite. When people talk
about politics with one another they negotiate the meanings of the competing
symbols and messages disseminated through such channels as the mass media.[12]
Politics is not limited to the topics on the agendas of the elite.
When talking about politics, people use their own experiences as guides
to thinking about what is fair.[13]
Talk about race can be talk about politics, especially when the topic of
discussion is racial stratification. American
society is racially stratified. Not
every racial and ethnic group has attained the same level of economic status.[14]
Racial stratification is an aspect of race relations which should
confront Americans of all races with the political question - Who gets what,
when, how?
In-depth
interviews enable one to study how people talk about racial stratification.
Although surveys of large numbers of respondents allow social scientists
to make generalizations about the general population, during surveys it is
difficult to delve deeply into the thoughts that people have. By allowing people to explain what their responses mean,
in-depth interviews fill in the gaps left by survey research and provide
qualitative data that surveys are unable to produce.
"If we want to understand how people make sense of the world of
politics, we need to spend more time talking to people about that world."[15]
A chief advantage of in-depth interviews is that they can be discursive.
Because respondents can ramble and follow their own trains of thought
during in-depth interviews, the researcher can gain insight into the connotative
meanings of words and phrases, follow the course of associative thinking, and
illuminate the mechanisms of argument and evasion employed by respondents in
dealing with sensitive political material.[16]
In-depth interviewing can also generate findings that survey research
does not. Where surveys find only
unexplained variance, in-depth interviews may find results because they allow
respondents to explain their response and thereby point out relationships among
variables which researchers may not expect to find or even imagine as possible.[17]
I
have constructed the following in-depth interview response from a series of
in-depth interviews which a graduate student undertook in the winter of 1993
with Whites, primarily of Russian ethnicity, who have resided in South Shore.
South Shore is a community area in Chicago where racial residential
succession from White to Black occurred during the 1960s and 1970s. The individuals whose interviews I focused on in constructing
the response all resided in South Shore prior to the time of racial succession.
The constructed response supports the strongest argument of my thesis:
talk about racial stratification conveys a convergence of responsibility and
racial meaning.
Every now and then,
particularly when I am with relatives or friends of mine who lived in South
Shore when I did, we talk about South Shore.
We talk about the friends we had, and all the places we used to go to
together, like to the beach, the park, and the shops in the area.
Sometimes I get kind of nostalgic. Even
though South Shore was in the city of Chicago, it had a warm and friendly
community feel to it when I lived there in the fifties.
People felt safe living there. It
was a white middle-class and upper middle-class community.
In the Jackson Park Highlands, you had very nice homes and very well-off
families.
These
days it's different. South Shore is
a Black neighborhood now. I don't
think you can call all of South Shore a middle-class neighborhood anymore. There is more crime and more of the types of problems you get
when there aren't enough middle-class people around. In some parts of South Shore I think life is terrible.
In these parts of South Shore, where you have more poor people, I think
they are having serious gang problems, drug problems, and crime problems.
We never had those kinds of problems.
If there was a fight, maybe like a fight between some rough types at
South Shore High school, well, then they fought with their fists. Today you're liable to get shot and killed by a thug if you
say the wrong thing to the wrong guy in South Shore.
There
was a man I knew from work who lives in South Shore.
Where this man lives, it's more or less middle class.
It's not as nice as the Jackson Park Highlands, but this man has a good
white collar job. He works very
hard and makes enough money to own a home.
He keeps it in pretty good shape. When
people own their homes they have more incentives than apartment dwellers to
maintain their homes in decent shape. You have to want to maintain your neighborhood in decent
shape if you want to live in a nice neighborhood.
I imagine it’s not completely safe in the neighborhood where this man
lives because I'm sure they get elements coming in there from other parts of
South Shore. Unfortunately, these
types who are just looking for trouble are always nearby. They make life hard for the
Black people in South Shore who are like this man.
There
are people I know from the neighborhood who have been near there for one reason
or another. Some of them have gone
back there to drive around the area and see what the neighborhood looks like.
Driving through the areas in South Shore where you have middle-class
Blacks homeowners, and especially driving through the Jackson Park Highlands,
it's not like you see the deterioration and blight of the inner city.
The middle-class Blacks in South Shore care about educating the children
and passing on a better life for the next generation, so they are not causing
the problems in South Shore. I'm
sure the middle-class people in South Shore are terrified of the crime in the
city just like everyone is. In the
parts of South Shore where there are some of the problems they have in the
ghetto, no one tells you from day one, "You are going to learn how to
behave, you must graduate high school, and you will improve yourself."
The Black people living in the inner city are stuck there in poverty
because they're hanging out on the corner buying, selling, and taking drugs.
You really can't do anything to help those people with welfare or any
kind of government program like that. They
need self-help. They are killing
each other over drugs instead of thinking about the future like middle class
people. Now, like I said, South
Shore is not the ghetto. But then,
it's very different from the way I remember it. Those days were a long time ago.
South Shore, and not just South Shore but the whole world, was different
when I was growing up.
How
did I construct and then interpret this response?
Meanings are in society and therefore in people.
In order to identify and interpret the racial meanings conveyed when
people talk about racial stratification, one needs to understand how the
individuals speaking are placed in the system of racial stratification.
Because not all individuals are presumed to be sociologists, one also
needs to know how the individuals speaking understand racial stratification in
the society in which they are situated. Central
to my methodology for identifying and interpreting the racial meanings conveyed
when people talk about racial stratification are assumptions about the role of
story telling in thinking and talking, as well as assumptions about the role of
responsibility in thinking and talking about problems and situations in society
such as racial stratification.
THE
RACIAL STRATIFICATION OF ECONOMIC STATUS, RACIAL RESIDENTIAL SUCCESSION, AND
RACIAL RESIDENTIAL SEGREGATION - THE AMERICAN EXPERIENCE
Income
statistics for 1990 (income as of March 1991) indicate that in the nation as a
whole Blacks occupy a lower economic status position than do Whites, Hispanics
(any race), and Asians and Pacific Islanders.
In 1990 the Black median family income was $21,423, only 58 percent of
the White median family income of $36,915.
The Hispanic median family income was $23,431, and the Asian and Pacific
Islander median family income was $42,245.
While 8.1 percent of White families, 10.7 percent of all Whites, and 15.1
percent of White children lived below the poverty level, 29.3 percent of Black
families, 31.9 percent of all Blacks, and 44.2 percent of Black children lived
below the poverty level. Twenty-five
percent of Hispanic families, 28.1 percent of all Hispanics, and 39.7 percent of
Hispanic children lived below the poverty level, as did 11.0 percent of Asian
and Pacific Islander families and 12.2 percent of all Asians and Pacific
Islanders.
Racial
residential segregation should be one item on the research agenda for the
comprehensive study of racial stratification and the relationship between race
and economics in the United States.[18]
How segregated is America? The
most widely accepted measure of residential segregation is the index of
dissimilarity. This index computes
the relative percentage of Blacks and Whites that would have to change their
neighborhood in order to achieve an even residential distribution in a larger
area.[19]
Indexes between 30 and 60 are considered to be moderate, and indexes
above 60 are considered to be high. The
average indexes for the 16 metropolitan areas with 250,000 or more Black
residents in 1980 were 81 in 1960, 83 in 1970, and 77 in 1980.[20]
Widespread and high levels of residential segregation continued unabated
into 1990. In 1990, the average
dissimilarity index for the 18 northern metropolitan areas with the largest
Black populations was 78, while the average dissimilarity index for the 12
southern metropolitan areas with the largest Black populations was 67.[21]
At the rate of change observed between 1970 and 1990, the average level
of Black-White segregation in the northern metropolitan areas would not reach
the lower bound of the high range until the year 2043.
At the slower rate of change prevailing from 1970 to 1980 it would take
until 2067. As of 1990, no northern
Black community approached a moderate level of segregation.[22]
In
1980, the relative levels of residential segregation of the 11 largest European
ethnic groups from the English, the "founding" European group,
generally reflected the length of time that these groups had been in the United
States. Although Blacks have lived in the United States longer than
any of the 11 largest European ethnic groups, Blacks were by far more segregated
from the English than were any of these European groups in 1980.[23]
Blacks are more segregated from Whites than are Hispanics and Asians,
many of whom are newly arrived immigrants.
If Black-White residential segregation scores were to follow the trends
of the 1970s and decline by 5 points each decade after 1980, Black residential
segregation would not fall to the 1980 levels of Hispanic and Asian residential
segregation until about the year 2040.[24]
The
relationship between racial residential segregation and class residential
segregation is an important topic of research not in the least because this
relationship has important consequences for the size and growth of concentrated
urban poverty among Blacks. One important hypothesis posits that the exodus of the Black
middle class from mixed income areas was a major factor in the growth of Black
concentrated urban poverty after 1970. According
to this hypothesis, these Blacks took with them a social buffer which had once
shielded poorer Blacks from some of the harmful social dislocations correlated
with the joblessness and poverty resulting from structural shifts in the
American economy.[25]
In contrast, another hypothesis posits that the increase in residential
segregation among Black social classes between 1970 and 1980 was not sufficient
to account for the rise in concentrated urban poverty among Blacks, and that the
interaction between rising poverty rates and high levels of segregation created
the urban underclass. According to this hypothesis, the structure of racial
residential segregation itself contributed to the rise in concentrated urban
poverty among Blacks, because an overall increase in the poverty rate of a
highly segregated group is accompanied by an increase in the concentration of
poverty for this group.[26]
Racial
succession is one of the mechanisms through which high levels of racial
residential segregation are maintained. Racial
succession is one form of residential succession, the replacement of one
population in an area by another. When
residential succession occurs, the initial population and its successor may
differ with respect to economic function, social status, ethnic or national
background, race, or other socially significant characteristics or combination
of characteristics. Racial
succession takes place when one racial category of the population replaces
another as residents of an area. In
an important work on residential succession and the Black population in Chicago
written during the 1950s, racial succession is described as a four stage process
of penetration, invasion, consolidation, and piling up.[27]
The bounded neighborhood model exemplifies how the theoretically
interesting relationship between the decision-making process of individuals and
the collective outcomes produced by the behavior of individuals has been an
important focus of research on racial succession.
The bounded neighborhood model explains how the pursuit of interest by
individual Blacks and Whites leads to segregation in a theoretical neighborhood
where most Whites would tolerate a small amount of integration and where there
is at first only a small influx of Black residents.[28]
A revised theory of neighborhood tipping, based on a survey of 8 Chicago
neighborhoods and data from several data sets designed to address specific
questions arising from the bounded neighborhood model, weighs the relative
importance of sociological factors such as racial prejudice, fear of crime, and
housing market factors as causes of racial turnover.
Tipping occurs when the concentration of racial minority residents in a
neighborhood reaches the tipping point, the level of racial minority
concentration which presages a complete racial turnover.
According to the revised theory of tipping, victimization, fear of crime,
and signs of neighborhood deterioration such as abandoned buildings or lack of
upkeep reinforce the cycle of structural weakness in housing markets, economic
dissatisfaction, and racial tipping.[29]
The persistence of discrimination in housing markets and white prejudice
leads to succession and the perpetuation of segregation.
"Through a series of exclusionary tactics, realtors limit the
likelihood of Black entry into white neighborhoods and channel Black demand for
housing into areas within or near existing ghettos.
White prejudice is such that when Black entry into a neighborhood is
achieved, that area becomes unattractive to further white settlement and whites
begin departing at an accelerated rate."[30]
THE
CHICAGO EXPERIENCE
The
Chicago experience, therefore, tends to refute any attempt to compare Northern
Negroes with European immigrants. Unlike
the Irish, Poles, Jews, or Italians, Negroes banded together not to enjoy a
common linguistic, cultural, and religious tradition, but because a systematic
pattern of segregation left them no alternative.[31]
The
outlines of the Black area of residence in Chicago were established by 1920.[32]
White hostility and racial violence played an important role first in the
creation and solidification of Chicago's Black Belt pattern of segregation, and
later in the transformation of the Black Belt, once a narrow strip of land
extending south from downtown, into the South Side area of Black residence.
Guerilla warfare and bombings between 1917 and 1919 gave way to open
armed conflict in 1919, housing riots in the 1940s and 1950s, and a continuous
atmosphere of fear, distrust, and racial incidents afterwards. The demographic pressures of the migration of Southern Blacks
into the city, Blacks' creation of an institutional ghetto in response to white
hostility, decades of restrictive covenants, racial succession, and the racial
policies of the Chicago Housing Authority all contributed to the transformation
of the Black Belt into the South Side area of Black residence.[33]
"As civil rights forces mobilized in the South, and the Brown v.
Board of Education of Topeka, Kansas decision of 1954 was hailed as a
possible new beginning in American race relations, Chicago moved in the opposite
direction by institutionalizing a greatly enlarged Black ghetto and admonishing
potential newcomers to stay away."[34]
Housing riots attest to the fact that racial residential segregation was
a norm in Chicago which many White Chicagoans were willing to fight to maintain.
Racial divisions centering on housing continued after the 1950s.
Martin Luther King led civil rights marches into White neighborhoods in
1966. The physical division of
Whites and Blacks in different electoral wards continued, establishing the
dominating context of racial animosity which set the stage in 1983 for the
election of Harold Washington as the city's first Black mayor, after a campaign
that was fought as a "race war Chicago style."[35]
The
results from a study of 203 metropolitan areas in which at least 4 percent of
the population was Black in 1980 ranked Chicago the fourth most segregated
metropolitan area in the nation. The
index in the Chicago Metropolitan Area was 88, and only Gary, Indiana Fort
Myers, Florida and Cleveland, Ohio were more segregated than Chicago.[36]
Chicago is an area of hypersegregation for Blacks.[37]
In 1990 Black, Hispanic, and Asian indexes were 86, 63, and 43
respectively.[38]
As
in the rest of the nation, the racial stratification of economic status is
considerable in Chicago. In 1990,
Chicago was the nation's third most populated city with 2,783,726 residents.
After New York City, Chicago had the largest Black population. The population in the city was 45.4 percent White, 39.1
percent Black, 19.6 percent Hispanic, and 3.7 percent Asian or Pacific Islander.
While the per capita income of Whites in 1989 was $18,258, the per capita
income of Blacks was only $8,569. The
per capita income of Asian and Pacific Islanders was $11,581, and the per capita
income of Hispanics was $7,438. Blacks
and Hispanics are by far more concentrated within the city limits than are
Asians and Pacific Islanders or Whites. In
the Chicago PMSA during 1990 30.1 percent of Whites, 81.7 percent of Blacks,
45.4 percent of Asians and Pacific Islanders, and 76.2 percent of Hispanics were
city residents. Per capita incomes
in the Chicago PMSA in 1989 were higher than those in the city, but show the
same general pattern of stratification as per capita incomes in the city:
$19,777 Whites; $9,205 Blacks; $14,754 Asians and Pacific Islanders; $8,264
Hispanics. The all-person poverty
rates in the Chicago PMSA for the four groups in 1989 were: 5.7 percent Whites;
29.5 percent Blacks; 10.0 percent Asians or Pacific Islanders; 20.5 percent
Hispanics.[39]
THE
SOUTH SHORE CASE
People
are both unique individuals and social beings functioning at specific historical
periods and locations in the social structure.
External or contextual realities constrain one's unique modes of
activity, but how one interprets or constructs reality depends upon the
uniqueness of one's personality.[40]
An exhaustive history could be written about the intersections between
prevailing ideologies, the unique experiences and idiosyncratic modes of
explanation of the individuals interviewed, and the specific historical patterns
of racial stratification in the nation, in Chicago, and in the particular
communities within and near Chicago where the individuals who were interviewed
have resided. I do not intend to
explicate fully this history, for instead, I discuss why it is significant that
South Shore is case of racial stratification which these individuals have though
about in the past.
South
Shore is one of the seventy-five "natural community areas" delimited
in the 1920s by researchers at the University of Chicago and institutionalized
in the Local Community Fact Book. The
concept of a natural community area involved both the ecological or spatial
patterns of communities, and the normative dimensions of community - the
cultural life, modes of living, customs, and standards.[41]
In Symbolic Communities, Hunter assesses the correspondence
between the social areas of social area analysis or factorial ecology and the
natural community areas. He also explores transformations in the ecology, symbolic
culture, and social structure of local urban communities brought about by the
increasing scale of modern urban societies.
Because the symbols of community names and boundaries may be readily
communicated and disseminated from authoritative sources of information and then
used by residents in their own definitions of communities, Burgess and his
colleagues, in delimiting the natural community areas, may have contributed to
the persistence of the community areas as socially meaningful entities.
However, the fusion and differentiation of community areas and the
establishment of status hierarchies of communities occurred, in part, because
community boundaries and names served inclusive and exclusive functions.
Residents of higher status communities consciously or unconsciously drew
sharp cognitive boundaries that excluded residents of adjacent lower status
communities, while the latter "blurred" these boundaries to include
themselves in the higher status communities.
Community organizations and other authoritative sources of information
assisted this process of fusion and differentiation by recognizing and even
creating revised community boundaries.
One
dispute between Molotch and Guest and Zuiches centered on South Shore community
area boundaries and the exclusion of Blacks living in two census tracts west of
Stony Island from South Shore by White residents, community organizations, and
by Molotch. With respect to the boundaries of South Shore, Molotch notes
"The most basic finding is that something called a 'South Shore' as a
distinct area does exist...The boundaries of South Shore are not unanimously
viewed, but there is widespread agreement-especially on three of the four
boundaries."[42]
According to Molotch, the three agreed upon boundaries are clear cut
paths and edges: Lake Michigan in the east, 67th Street in the north, and Stony
Island in the west.[43]
Molotch conducted his dissertation research (later published in Managed
Integration) between 1965 and 1967 on residential turnover in South Shore
and on the efforts of the South Shore Commission to manage integration.
According to Molotch, South Shore underwent dramatic racial change
between 1960 and 1966. By 1965,
when Molotch's study of South Shore began, a large number of Black residents,
close to a fourth of the total population of the area Molotch defined as South
Shore, were concentrated in the northwest corner of the community.
According to Molotch, the first Black families are said to have moved
into this corner of South Shore at 67th and Stony Island in 1958.
Most white residents dated the onset of racial change with 1960 and saw
succession, moving to the south and east as the Black Belt expanded, as an
unstoppable process. However, based
on his comparison of residential turnover in South Shore and Rogers Park,
Molotch argues that racial residential succession in South Shore during the
1960s does not constitute "White flight."[44]
Instead, a stable process of residential turnover took place due to the
dual housing market. Housing starved Blacks were willing to pay higher rents for
housing in South Shore than were Whites who had numerous attractive housing
opportunities available to them elsewhere.
Consequently, White migration into South Shore was insufficient to
counter-balance the in-migration of Blacks.
The declining number of Whites and the increasing number of Blacks was
interpreted by remaining White residents as evidence of the failure of managed
integration.
Guest
and Zuiches claimed that Molotch's definition of South Shore was
methodologically unsound for the purposes of his analysis of residential
turnover in South Shore. They
claimed that compared to the Burgess Fact Book boundaries, Molotch's
South Shore boundaries excluded two census tracts which were already heavily
Black by 1960. According to Fact
Book boundaries, nearly 10 percent of the South Shore population was Black
in 1960.[45]
In reply to the criticism that his boundaries simply made official the
psychological de-annexation of Black areas from South Shore by South Shore
residents, Molotch claims his dissertation research, which involved participant
observation, interviews with local residents of South Shore, and the examination
of church, school, and organizational boundaries, justified his exclusion of the
two predominantly Black census tracts (to the west of Stony Island) from his
definition of South Shore. Molotch
argues that these tracts were never considered part of South Shore by South
Shore residents or community boosters, even when the area west of Stony Island
was White. Molotch also places the
southern boundary for South Shore south of the Fact Book boundary.
Arguing that South Shore boundaries (throughout the history of the area)
have been perceived by South Shore residents as different from the Fact Book
boundaries, Molotch notes, "Fact Book boundaries originated in the
1930s and have no analytical force apart from their conformity to the
conceptions of community which people actually hold."[46]
In Managed Integration, Molotch acknowledges that differentiation
based on race may have played a role between 1940 and 1965 in the exclusion of
the area west of Stony Island from South Shore by the indigenous White
population and the South Shore Commission, but he nonetheless defends his
adoption of the South Shore Commission's boundaries for South Shore.[47]
Manley
argues in By the Color of Their Skins that even after succession was
complete the Black class structure in South Shore resembled the White
counterpart, that differences are a product of industrial development and racial
and class contradictions, and that racial contradictions explain the differences
between Black and White class structures. In
unsuccessfully attempting to exclude Blacks from South Shore and manage
integration, Whites judged incoming Blacks by the color of their skins and not
by the content of their characters. During
the 1960s Blacks entering South Shore were equal to those Whites leaving in
terms of education. Blacks were
slightly more educated in the 1970s. After
South Shore became a predominantly Black neighborhood, the incomes of many of
these Blacks in South Shore increased. However,
the age structure of the Black population differed from that of the White
population. Due to the increase of
the younger age group which corresponded to the national trend of Black
adolescent unemployment and out of wedlock births, the median income decreased
as a percentage of White median income in South Shore during the seventies.[48]
In Against the Tide: The Middle Class in Chicago, Camacho and
Joravsky characterize South Shore as a middle-class community that is
"coming back" due to both the efforts of the South Shore Bank which
has channeled millions of private, local, state, and federal dollars into the
community for investment and rehabilitation, and the efforts of individuals like
a Black social worker profiled by the authors.
His middle-class values led him to rehab his own home and work for
community improvement.[49]
Whatever
boundaries one chooses, racial succession did occur in South Shore.
As Table 1 below shows, over the course of the thirty years from 1950 to
1980, the population in South Shore (Fact Book boundaries) changed from
99.8 percent White in to 95.1 percent Black.
Total
Population and Percent White, Black, and Other Non-White in
Year
Population %
White
% Black
% Other Non-White
1950
79,336
99.8
0.2
-
1960
79,086
89.6
9.6
0.8
1970
80,660
29.9
69.0
1.1
1980
77,743
3.6
95.1
1.3
1990
61,517
2.0
97.5
0.7
SOURCES:
U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1990 Census of Population and Housing, Summary Tape
Files CD90-1A-3-2 and CD90-1C. The
Chicago Fact Book Consortium, Local Community Fact Book Chicago Metropolitan
Area, 1970 and 1980 pp. 34-36, 116-118. - = < 0.1%.
The
percentage of families living below the poverty line in all census tracts in
South Shore has increased considerably after 1970, although there are
considerable differences by census tract in both median family incomes and
family poverty rates in South Shore. In
1989, in only 5 of the 14 census tracts in South Shore was the median family
income above the city wide median family income of $26,301.
In 10 census tracts family poverty rates were higher than the city wide
rate of 18.3 percent.
Why
is it reasonable to assume that when the individuals interviewed for this thesis
spoke about the place, South Shore, they were thinking and talking about racial
stratification? Each individual believed something called a South Shore
exists, and each claimed to have resided in South Shore before racial
residential secession was complete. Follow-up
interviews conducted in January 1994 revealed that the 5 individuals profiled in
the appendix did recognize many, although not all, of the 11 neighborhood names
Molotch identified, and that they believed that they are part of South Shore.
This suggests that South Shore was meaningful to these individuals on the
normative dimension.[50]
Even decades after they physically moved from what they consider to be
South Shore, these individuals have thought and talked about this area,
maintaining a curiosity concerning the fate of the South Shore with which they
were familiar as children, adolescents, young adults, and adults.[51]
In the appendix, the profiles of the 5 individuals show that all 5 are
upper middle-class Whites, and that 4 reside in suburbs where the populations
were almost exclusively White in 1970. In
addition to the high levels of racial stratification in the nation and in
Chicago, these individuals' lingering curiosity concerning the condition of a
community where racial succession and increased poverty have occurred over the
same time period, and their own experiences as upper middle-class Whites living
by and large among other upper middle-class Whites, support the assumption that
these individuals have had good reason to think and talk about racial
stratification long before they were interviewed.
IDENTIFYING
THE RACIAL MEANINGS CONVEYED DURING IN-DEPTH INTERVIEWS
In
order to proceed with an analysis of the racial meanings conveyed the during
in-depth interviews I need a clearer definition of meaning.
Although the definition of meaning on page one of this thesis is
appropriate for the analysis of in-depth interview responses because it focuses
on communication through language, I will attempt to clarify this definition
through a comparison of the racial meanings I will identify to opinions,
attitudes, and schemata - constructs which have been used widely in survey
research.
The
crucial distinctions between opinions, attitudes, and schemata are a matter of
the different objectives and assumptions that guide the analysis of survey data
(theoretically, even the same data set) by researchers who study opinions,
attitudes, and schemata. The
absence of a consensus on what constitutes public opinion has led some writers
on public opinion to eschew definitions and to discuss the qualities of public
opinion. Opinions may be widely
distributed in the population or narrowly held by a few people, stable or
volatile, and held weakly or strongly. One
thing about opinions is certain. Opinions
are focused and expressed in terms of specific issues or problems.[52]
Policy preferences, evaluations of public officials, and the like that
large proportions of the population profess to hold, are examples of issue
opinions.[53]
Opinions are like coded messages about people's life experiences and are
the result of complex value and belief calculations that establish a sensible
fit between these experiences and the outer world.[54]
Compared to opinions, attitudes are more like the underlying systems of
beliefs and values providing the keys to the coded messages.
Attitudes are the broader, deeper feelings and fundamental beliefs which
are part of the basic personality of respondents.
Attitudes underlie opinions.[55]
They are the positive or negative responses individuals have toward some
object that underlie the responses expressed in surveys.[56]
Two
chief criticisms have been leveled against schema theory: measures of schemata
are really measures of attitudes, and the hypotheses derived by schema theory
could be easily derived by attitude theory.
In response to these criticisms some supporters of the schema concept
have argued that there are important differences between attitude and schema
theories.[57]
While affective response and motivational consistency are central to
attitude theory, the core of schema theory is the emphasis on hierarchical
categories, cognitive structures, and information processing.
Schemata lend organization to an individual's experience, structure the
way information is remembered, guide the inferring of new information, and
provide a basis for evaluations and problem solving.[58]
This is not to say that there is no place for affect in information
processing models using schema theory, such as in the cognitive-affective model
of the role of social groups in political thinking.
Schemata
and attitudes are hypothetical constructs created to assist explanations of
human behavior. "Thus no one has ever measured or will ever measure an
attitude directly. We can only
observe the empirical consequences of schemata and attitudes, not the schemata
and attitudes themselves."[59]
Meanings are not hypothetical constructs in the same way that attitudes
and schemata are hypothetical constructs. Communication
through language can consist of opinions, focused and expressed in terms of
specific issues or problems. Focused
and expressed opinions, and hence meanings, are more directly observable and
directly measurable than are schemata and attitudes.
However, meanings are also hypothetical, like schemata and attitudes,
because meanings tell us about what and how people think as well as what they
say. Communication through language
involves thinking and the processing of information. We cannot measure thinking and information processing
directly by listening to peoples' opinions.
Thinking, like schemata and attitudes, underlies focused and expressed
opinions. Racial meanings are
conveyed through language and can take the form of opinions, but racial meanings
are not simply the words, statements, anecdotes, stories, and opinions (the
empirical consequences of thinking) we "measure" directly when we hear
people speak or when we read interview transcripts.
Racial meanings are words and the thoughts, deeper significances, and
affect underlying words.
Racial
meanings underlie words and can be expressed through heavily nuanced and
symbolic language, but racial meanings are not covert or hidden.
Racial meanings are part focused opinion.
If something is covert, it cannot at the same time be expressed outright
as an opinion. This is a
methodological argument. Researchers
using in-depth interviews cannot use statistical inference to determine whether
or not a sample of the population exhibits a tendency to answer particular
questions covertly. "In
opinion polling, the researcher infers the links between variables; in
intensive interviewing, the researcher induces the respondent to create
the links between variables as he or she sees them."[60]
One does not openly create links between variables and at the same time
covertly conceal these links. If a
respondent is prejudiced against Blacks and does not want the researcher to know
about it, he or she will not say outright, "I am prejudiced against Blacks,
but since it is socially undesirable to be prejudiced against Blacks, I will
speak covertly."
The
racial meanings conveyed during in-depth interviews both underlie and are
focused opinions, but they are not something the researcher can infer.
Hence an important question remains to be answered.
How does one identify the racial meanings that are most clearly conveyed by
the respondents themselves during in-depth interviews?
My answer to this question is based on a set of assumptions about the
role of storytelling in thinking and talking, and about the role of
responsibility in thinking about problems and situations in society such as
racial stratification.
Stories
are basic to the human thinking process. Because
people think and understand the world in terms of stories, the stories told
during in-depth interview responses are clues to how respondents think about and
understand some aspect of the world. The
gist of a story tells us most about the meaning, including the racial meaning,
conveyed in a story. People have
dynamic memories which change in response to what they learn about the world
with the passing of time. These
assumptions about stories, gists, and memory are adapted from a work on the role
of stories in human and artificial intelligence:
The
central premise here is that a data base of partial stories rather than whole
ones exists in memory. After a period of time, one remembers first- or secondhand
experiences as a residue of partial stories.
To tell a story, then, some set of events, together with a
characterization of things that have been learned from those events and a
characterization of the open questions in one's mind to which those events
relate, are concatenated into a baseline representation, which we call the story
basis or gist of a story. Gists
are structured sets of events that function as a single unit in memory that can
be transformed by a variety of processes into actual stories.
Each time a story is told, its gist is accessed and manipulated for a
particular purpose. A gist is a
dynamic entity which can change or be replaced over time by adding or deleting
details in subsequent telling...The only things we remember [when we tell a
story] are the gists that we access.[61]
Story
creation is a memory process. When
a person first tells a story he or she formulates the gist of an experience and
creates the memory structure that will contain the gist of the story for the
rest of his or her life.[62]
When a person re-tells a story he or she consciously or unconsciously
adds to or subtracts from the gist in memory the details, truths, untruths, and
embellishments which transform the gist into an actual story communicating the
gist. The accuracy of the details
in a spoken response is therefore less important than how these details clarify
the gist. The gist is what is stored, remembered, and communicated.
The details assist communication by transforming the gist into a story
the story teller and other people can understand.
Although gists are transformed and manipulated when stories are actually
told, gists remain the baseline representations of stories which are the clues
to how people think about and understand some aspect of the world.
Story-based knowledge expresses the storyteller's point of view and
philosophy of life that comes from experiences. However, story-based memory is dynamic because memory is
transformed by experience, learning, and the re-telling of old stories.
I
do not assume that any of the individuals who were interviewed could accurately
recall all of the details of his or her own life, and certainly not the details
of the lives of other people. I do
not assume that these individuals provided accurate sociological analyses of
racial stratification. Nonetheless,
the gists of their responses are worth studying because they are the keys to
understanding what these individuals meant when they spoke about racial
stratification. The details of the responses, accurate or inaccurate, are
important to the extent that they clarify the racial meanings conveyed in the
gists of the responses.
Arguing
that a lack of empirical evidence for global theories has led public opinion
researchers to turn to more idiosyncratic conceptualizations of political
opinionation, Iyengar has advanced a domain-specific theory of public opinion in
which "the primary factor that determines opinions concerning political
issues is the assignment of responsibility for the issue in question; that is,
individuals tend to simplify political issues by reducing them to questions of
responsibility, and their opinions on issues flow from their answers to these
questions."[63]
Founded upon evidence which is primarily from nonpolitical domains, such
as psychological research concerning the simplifying and heuristic-like role of
responsibility in everyday reasoning, the premise of Iyengar's theory is that
people think about responsibility instinctively and that attributions of
responsibility are critical ingredients of all social knowledge.[64]
"By reducing the complexity of political issues to the twin concepts
of causation and treatment, attributions (of responsibility) enable citizens to
structure the otherwise dazzling array of events, policies, institutions,
groups, and personalities that make up the day-to-day substance of national
politics."[65]
I draw upon the following ideas from Iyengar's work.
Political thinking involves the responsibility heuristic applied in
specific domains or contexts, and global concerns.
The latter can be ideology and the conflicting capitalist
(individualistic) and democratic (egalitarian) values of the "American
ethos" which are supported by elites and opinion leaders who disseminate
political norms through various media as parts of ideologically integrated
packages.[66]
These global concerns influence the way people think about the role of
the individual and the role of society in treating and causing problems.
CONCLUSION:
RACIAL MEANING IN THE CONSTRUCTED RESPONSE
The
interviews conducted for this thesis provided individuals with numerous
opportunities to comment on the problems and situations arising from racial
stratification. It is impossible to
fit neatly their responses into a scheme of responsibility such as the one
Iyengar uses.[67]
However, in order to identify the racial meanings conveyed by these
individuals, I studied the gists of people's stories, looking for what these
gists conveyed about who is held responsible for causing racial stratification
and the problems associated with it. The
response is one I have constructed from the gists of many of the responses of
the individuals profiled in the appendix. One
constructed response does not adequately represent all of the varied and
contradictory responses given by the individuals interviewed.
However, this response represents one form of the convergence of
responsibility and racial meaning that formed a large portion of the responses -
the individualistic and affect-driven convergence of responsibility and racial
meaning.
In
order to identify the racial meanings expressed in the response I relate the
details of the response to the gists, focusing on the attributions of
responsibility, which are like the morals of the gists.
The details in the response are mainly descriptions of the several
objects that are compared and opposed by the "respondent."
These objects are places or people, or combinations of both, existing at
specified periods in time. For
example, one such object is "the middle-class white who resided in South
Shore during the 1950s." Another
object is "the middle-class Black who resides in South Shore during the
1990s." Sometimes the
respondent describes the objects. Other
times the respondent clearly implies that the objects exist.
The response conveys racial meaning through the attributions of
responsibility in the story gists that lie behind comparisons and oppositions of
objects. At times the respondent
explicitly compares and opposes the objects.
At other times we must juxtapose descriptions of the objects in order to
piece together the story about responsibility.
It
is important to reiterate that questions about the accuracy of the details are
not central to my analysis of racial meaning in the response.
In order to analyze racial meaning in the response it is not necessary to
assume that the object descriptions are founded upon an accurate sociological
analysis of South Shore or on an understanding of the history of racial
stratification in Chicago. My
fundamental argument is that there is a convergence of responsibility and racial
meaning when people talk about racial stratification, not that all people are
experts, and not that all people would give response such as the one given by
this "respondent." Outsiders'
distortions of facts (and the individuals interviewed, no longer residing in
South Shore, are outsiders) are to be expected.
Having undergone racial change, South Shore is viewed as a mysterious,
dangerous, depressing area by the people who do not reside there.[68]
The distortions must be taken along with the rest of the response and
searched for meaning.
The
story about responsibility is undeveloped in the beginning of the response.
The two main objects described are the entire South Shore community area
during the 1950s, as well as a more specified neighborhood within this community
area, the Jackson Park Highlands during the 1950s.
The Jackson Park Highlands are set apart as wealthier than the rest of
middle-class and upper middle-class South Shore.[69]
The respondent first looks back upon the South Shore of the 1950s with
nostalgia. This was a warm and
friendly place full of middle-class and upper middle-class families.
Although South Shore was located in the city of Chicago, the South Shore
of the 1950s has a community feel to it and people felt safe living there.
The respondent has fond memories of this place, its amenities, and its
people.
The
story about responsibility begins to develop as other objects are described.
The respondent is certain that the South Shore of today is different from
the way it was in the past. The
initial distinction, the one most clear to the respondent, is one between the
White South Shore of the past and the Black South Shore of today.
Whereas the former was middle class, the latter is no longer
characterized as middle class by the respondent.
Opposed to the nostalgically remembered South Shore of the 1950s is the
terrible life in the parts of South Shore where there are more poor people.
The South Shore of the 1950s is remembered for its beaches, parks, shops,
and safe, friendly atmosphere. The
South Shore of the 1990s is said to have serious gang, drug, and crime problems.
The response appears to be headed in the direction of individualistic
dissatisfaction with the changes that have taken place in South Shore.
Individuals are singled out as responsible for these changes.
These are people such as the "thugs" who shoot upon being
insulted. However, at this point
the response could take on a very different aspect as attributions of
responsibility are directed elsewhere.
Although
there is an association made between there being an increase in poor Black
people in South Shore, and more drug, gang, and crime problems, there are as of
yet no absolutely clear attributions of responsibility.
Poor Blacks in South Shore are not said to be responsible for gang, drug,
and crime problems. We cannot yet
fully understand the racial meaning conveyed by the respondent's association of
Black, poor, and social problems because we do not know who or what the
respondent thinks is causally responsible for this association.
With
respect to the cause of this association, the response could continue from here
basically in two broad directions. The
respondent could attribute causal responsibility for these problems to the
faults of individuals. Following
this direction, the poor people who live in South Shore where "life is
terrible" would be seen as responsible due to such failings as a deficient
work ethic or low social interest for problems such as gangs, drugs, and crime.
The respondent could also attribute the cause of these problems to
society. Such factors as
discrimination or the economy could be deemed responsible for Black poverty.
The last three sentences of the second paragraph of the response appear
to hint at the direction which the response will follow.
The blunt quality to the statement "we never had those
problems," and the graphic quality of the description of what suffices to
get oneself shot and killed in South Shore today, both suggest that the
respondent is expressing anger at these changes the respondent believes have
taken place. Such anger could be an
indication of the respondent's affective response towards Blacks in South Shore.
However, as of yet we do not know for certain where this anger is
directed. On the one hand, it could
be directed at a class of individuals, such as poor Blacks who live in South
Shore, who are seen to be responsible for social problems in South Shore.
On the other hand, the anger could be directed at the discriminatory
society or government which has neglected its responsibility to see that Blacks
living in South Shore are provided with opportunities to live in neighborhoods
like the nostalgically remembered White South Shore.
Some combination of both is also possible.
The
next objects described are types of people whom the respondent believes to live
in South Shore now. Once again, more important than the accuracy of the details
in this response is how the details clarify the gist. Whether or not the respondent actually knew a co-worker who
is a middle-class Black man who resides in South Shore, the manner in which this
man and his neighborhood are described is revealing. The Black man described in this response is a crystallization
of some of the characteristics of the middle class in the form of a story about
an individual. He works at his
white collar job, and hence makes enough money to own his own home.
His hard work and efforts to maintain his home in decent shape stand in
contrast both to the neglect of apartment dwellers to maintain their property
and the criminal efforts of the elements coming into his neighborhood from other
parts of South Shore. While the
former is a hard working and maintaining his property in decent condition, the
latter are looking for trouble. We
now have a clearer story about responsibility.
Individual efforts lead to home ownership, property maintenance, and a
nice neighborhood. The trouble
makers are the ones who are responsible for making life hard for the middle
class. However, the story is not
yet complete. Why do the trouble
makers make trouble? As of now we
only know that they are poor and Black. Considering
the association of Black, poor, and crime, combined with the affect conveyed in
the response, it appears more certain that the respondent may favor an
individualistic view of responsibility. However,
the association of poverty and Black with crime could very well be based on a
societal explanation for causal responsibility. That is, in response to the deprivations imposed by a society
which allows poverty and discrimination to flourish, poor Black people turn to
crime, making life hard for middle-class Blacks who, despite the setbacks
imposed by discrimination, are trying to work hard for the benefit of themselves
and their communities.
In
the final section of the response the story about responsibility is more fully
developed. Once again the Jackson
Park Highlands stand out as a point of comparison.
White or Black, the area is recognized as the wealthiest are in South
Shore. Why, in the Jackson Park Highlands and in the middle-class
areas of South Shore, is there not the blight and deterioration of the inner
city? Because middle-class and
upper middle-class Blacks care about the future of their children and instilling
in them a desire and ability for self-improvement. While South Shore is distinct from the inner city, the inner
city is another point of comparison in the response due to the similarities
between poor Blacks in South Shore and inner city Blacks. The story about responsibility reaches its full development
with the statement "They have to want to help themselves."
Here the respondent could have said "Even though individuals are
responsible for creating problems in South Shore, society is responsible for
finding and implementing the solutions to the problems."
Instead the response is more purely individualistic.
Individuals are responsible for both the causes and solutions of the
problems associated with racial stratification. The last statement shows that the respondent does not view
the changes in South Shore as an isolated development, but part of wider changes
in American society.
What
is the racial meaning of the respondent's individualist attributions of
responsibility? The simple answer
is that the respondent is a White looking back upon a Black neighborhood which
has undergone social change and hence the respondent holds Blacks individually
responsible for these changes. This
answer does not suffice because a response constructed from interviews of
middle-class Blacks who reside in South Shore would probably look very similar
to the constructed response in this thesis.
Intra-class divisions between middle-class and upper middle-class Blacks
living in Parkside and the Jackson Park Highlands in South Shore are fused
because another clearer division exists. Middle-class
and upper middle-class Blacks in South Shore view the underclass as a serious
"element" to be reckoned with in terms of crime and low social
interest.[70]
As a middle-class Black social worker from South Shore has said,
"To
me, middle-class values have to do with a belief in a system, a belief that it
is possible to get ahead; a belief that hard work pays off.
A lot of people in the underclass no longer believe in the system.
Their attitude is: Why go to High School?
Why work when I can get money doing other things?"[71]
Both
the White respondent looking back on South Shore from the standpoint of a former
resident, and the middle-class Black who actually lives there, believe in a
system of middle-class values. Hence
the racial meaning of the constructed response is not identical with
individualism, because individualism is raceless.
This is true despite the differences in the respondent's individualism
and the Black social worker's individualism.
The former's individualism is complete.
He says "They (poor Blacks in South Shore) need self-help."
They are responsible for getting themselves into poverty, and they are
responsible for getting themselves out of poverty.
In contrast the Black social worker says, that the American Dream is
perverted. "It's always what
can I do for me. Well, what about
the common good?" His
individualism has a softer aspect to it. Although
the Black poor are experiencing problems because they lack middle-class values,
the Black social worker sees it as society's responsibility to correct these
problems. For him there is a common
good which takes precedence over individualism.
The
racial meaning of the constructed response is apparent when we isolate and
connect lines such as,
We
never had those kinds of problems. If
there was a fight, maybe like a fight between some rough types at South Shore
High school, well, then they fought with their fists.
Today you're liable to get shot and killed by a thug if you say the wrong
thing to the wrong guy in South Shore.
and,
You
really can't do anything to help those people with welfare or any kind of
government program like that. They
need self-help. They are killing
each other over drugs instead of thinking about the future like middle-class
people.
The
real racial meaning in the constructed response lies in the manner in which
individualism and affect are linked. In
the response there is a frustrated, even angered nostalgia for the old days when
South Shore was White, and also a firm, unbending individualism.
Perhaps anger directed at the failure of a group of individuals to
perform their "duty" to help themselves is less diffuse than anger
directed at the amorphous entity, society.
Hence the solidity of the individualism in the response raises the level
of affect, and vice versa.
Racial
stratification (the racial stratification of economic status, racial succession,
and racial residential segregation) is national in scope, and Chicago is an
extremely racially stratified city and metropolitan area.
Prior to the interviews the individuals who were interviewed had good
reason to think and talk about racial stratification as they attempted to make
sense of the intersections between their own life experiences and racial
stratification in the nation, in Chicago, and in the smaller communities in
Chicago and elsewhere where these individuals have resided.
Because thinking, talking, and story telling are connected together in a
memory process, these individuals had strongly held, or at least previously
considered, story gists about racial stratification stored in memory.
The in-depth interviews are a good source of information on racial
meanings because they elicited previously held story gists about the pervasive
problem of racial stratification. The
constructed response illustrates how talk about racial stratification conveys a
convergence of responsibility and racial meaning.
During
the winter and spring of 1993 I interviewed 22 White individuals who can be
divided into three groups. The
first group includes five individuals who previously resided in Albany Park, a
community area in Chicago where racial residential succession has been on-going
since 1970, but not from White to Black. In
this masters thesis I have focused on the meanings conveyed when upper
middle-class Whites talk about Black-White stratification.
Consequently I have not focused on the interviews of these individuals.
The second group consists of 17 individuals who have resided in South
Shore. In creating the constructed
response I focused on the interviews responses of a third group consisting of
five individuals in the second group who were most distant from the interviewer.
Prior to the interviews these 5 did not know the interviewer directly or
indirectly.
The following are profiles of the 5 individuals whose interviews I have
focused on in constructing the constructed response.
Short follow up interviews were conducted in January 1994 in order to
collect additional information required for the profiles.
Income is each individual's estimated or "ball park" figure.
For married individuals it is the estimated combined income of the
individual and his or her spouse. "Moved
to suburb" refers to the suburb where the individual resided at the time of
the interview. Information about
these suburbs is purposely vague to protect the respondent’s identities, but
specific enough to illustrate that these are high income suburbs which were all
more than 90 percent White in 1980. Demographic
details are not entirely accurate and ages and dates are not necessarily exact,
again to protect the respondents’ identities.
Respondent
Profiles
Male 1
Age:
57
Religion:
Jewish
Education:
Some College
White
collar occupation.
Has
children.
1993
income: over $100,000
Moved
from South Shore in 1959.
Moved
to suburb in 1978.
1989
median household income of suburb: over $70,000.
Race
of suburb 1990: More than 80% White, more than 10% Black.
Race
of suburb 1980: More than 90% White, less than 5 percent Black.
Race
of suburb 1970: More than 99% White.
Female 1
Age:
58
Religion:
Jewish
Education:
College
Retired
teacher.
Married,
has children.
1993
income: Respondent refused to provide information about income.
Moved
from South Shore to suburb in 1957.
Describes
her current residence as an expensive condominium in Chicago.
Describes
her husband as a successful white collar professional.
Female 2
Age:
60
Religion:
Catholic
Education:
College
White
collar occupation.
Married,
has children.
1993
income: 90,000.
Moved
from South Shore in 1960.
Moved
to suburb in 1972.
1989
median household income: between $45,000 and $55,000.
Race
of suburb 1990: More than 90% White, less than 2% Black.
Race
of suburb 1980: More than 90% White.
Male 2
Age:
60
Religion:
Jewish
Education:
Advanced degree.
Teacher.
Married,
has children.
1993
income: $100,000.
Moved
from South Shore in 1964.
Moved
to suburb in 1972.
1989
median household income of suburb: between $45,000 and $55,000.
Race
of suburb 1990: More than 90% White, more than 5% Black.
Race
of suburb 1980: More than 95% White.
Race
of suburb 1970: More than 99 percent White.
Female 3
Age:
56
Religion:
Jewish
Education:
Advanced degree.
White
collar occupation.
Married,
no children.
1993
income: over $100,000.
Moved
from South Shore in 1967.
Move
to suburb in 1967.
1989
median household income of suburb: over $70,000.
Race
of suburb 1990: More than 95% White, less than 2 percent Black.
Race
1980: More than 95% White.
Race
1970: More than 99% White.
INTERVIEW
QUESTIONS
What
are the first few thoughts or feelings which come to your mind right now when
you think of South Shore?
Are
these the same kinds of thoughts or feelings which usually come to your mind
when you think of South Shore? (IF
NO) How are they different?
Imagine
that you have a very close friend who has never heard of South Shore. Your friend wants to know what South Shore was like when you
lived there. What would you say to
your friend?
What
are the first few thoughts or feelings which come to your mind right now when
you think of Suburbtown?
Are
these the same kinds of thoughts or feelings which usually come to your mind
when you think of Suburbtown? (IF
NO) How are they different?
Imagine
that your very close friend has never heard of Suburbtown.
Your friend wants to know what it is like to live in Suburbtown.
What would you say to your friend?
Imagine
this same friend wants to know what it is like to live in South Shore today.
Even though you don't live there anymore, what would you say to your
friend?
Do
you ever get together with family or friends who lived in South Shore when you
did?
When
you are with these people do you ever reminisce about your lives in South Shore?
What
specifically do you talk about when you talk about South Shore with these
people?
When
you are talking about South Shore with these people, do you feel pleasant,
happy, sad, or upset?
Why
do you think you feel that way?
When
did you move out of South Shore?
Why
did you decide to move out of South Shore?
Why
did you move to Suburbtown (or place moved to directly after moving from South
Shore)? Why did you move there
rather than somewhere else?
What
were the houses and apartments like on your block in South Shore?
Was
your block like most blocks in South Shore?
How was it different?
Were
your parents (or guardians if applicable) renters or home owners? Later, did you live outside of your parents' household in
South Shore? Were you a renter or a
home owner then?
What
were your neighbors like in South Shore? Characterize
your relationship with your neighbors. Did
you consider your neighbors to be mainly strangers, acquaintances, friends,
close friends, or like family? Was
your family friendly with many other families on your block, a few, or none?
Were
you a member of a temple or church congregation when you lived in South Shore? How
would you characterize this temple or church and your participation in religious
and social activities at or through this temple or church?
Did
your involvement in religious or social activities change after you moved from
South Shore? How would you
characterize these changes?
Think
back to your childhood and your three or four closest friends in elementary or
grammar school. Where did they live
and what were they like? Were their
backgrounds a lot like yours or different?
How so?
What
did you and your friends like to do for fun when you were in elementary or
grammar school?
What
about your friends in high school? What
were they like?
What
did you like to do for fun and where did you socialize in high school?
How
did the high school you attended compare to other high schools in Chicago at the
time? What were the students and
teachers like there?
In
your opinion, what are some of the main differences between life in South Shore
when you lived there, and life in Suburbtown today?
What
are your neighbors like in Suburbtown? Characterize
your relationship with your neighbors. Do
you consider your neighbors to be mainly strangers, acquaintances, friends,
close friends, or like family? Is
your family friendly with many other families on your block, a few, or none?
What
are your friends like in Suburbtown? What
are their backgrounds like? Where
do they live in relation to where you live?
Compared
to the friends you had in South Shore, are these people more like you or less
like you?
What
are some of your feelings about these differences or similarities?
Do
you have any children? What do you
think it was like for your children to grow up in Suburbtown?
What were their closest friends like?
What kinds of families and backgrounds did they come from?
In what ways were they different or similar?
What
kind of high school did your children attend in Suburbtown?
What were the students and teachers like there?
What do you think the students do for fun?
Do
you think your children missed out on something valuable because they grew up in
Suburbtown and not in South Shore? Did
you have any valuable experiences growing up in South Shore that they did not
have because they grew up in Suburbtown? What
were some of these experiences?
In
your opinion, what are some of the main differences between life in South Shore
when you lived there and life in South Shore today?
What are the people like who live in South Shore today?
How are they different from or similar to you?
How
has South Shore (or Hyde Park) High School changed over the years?
What is it like today? What
are the students and teachers like there? How
does it compare to schools in the suburbs? How does it compare to the way you remember it?
What
accounts for any of the changes in South Shore?
When
was the last time you visited or drove through or near your old neighborhood?
What were you doing?
Describe
some of your impressions of South Shore when you last visited it. How did you feel about what you saw?
Do
you or would you feel comfortable driving through South Shore in your car? (IF UNCOMFORTABLE?) Would
it depend upon where you drove to, the time, or both? Would it change things if someone else accompanied you?
What kind of person would you want to accompany you in order to feel
comfortable? Why do you or would
you feel this way?
How
about eating in a restaurant or taking a walk in a park you visited regularly as
a child? Would it depend upon the
time or who accompanied you?
Does
this situation affect you or has it shaped your feelings or beliefs about
anything in any way? How so?
Do
you think that there is anything wrong about the way South Shore changed since
you moved away? Is it anyone's
fault? Do you think anything should
be or can be done to change the way South Shore is today?
What can be done? Whose
responsibility is it to do it?
Is
there anything else concerning either the past or present South Shore about
which you would like to comment?
SOURCES
Bennett,
Lance W.
Public Opinion in American Politics. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich,
1980.
Burgess,
Ernest W., and Donald J. Bogue
"Research in Urban Society: A Long View." In Ernest W. Burgess and
Donald J. Bogue, eds. Urban Sociology. Chicago: The University of Chicago
Press, 1967 [1964].
Camacho,
Eduardo, and Ben Joravsky
Against the Tide: The Middle Class in Chicago. Chicago: Community Renewal
Society, 1989.
Conover,
Pamela Johnston
"The Role of Social Groups in Political Thinking." British Journal
of Political Science 18 (1988):51-76.
Drake,
St. Clair, and Horace R. Clayton
Black Metropolis. New York: Harper, 1962 (1945).
Du
Bois, W. E. B.
The Souls of Black Folk. Chicago: A. C. McClurg and Company, 1911.
Du
Bois, W.E.B.
"The
Problem of Problems" The Intercollegiate Socialist December-January
1917-1918, pp. 5-9 in Philip S. Foner, ed., W.E.B. Du Bois Speaks. New
York: Pathfinder, 1970.
Duncan,
Dudley Otis, and Beverly Duncan
The
Negro Population of Chicago. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1957.
Edelman,
Murray
The Symbolic Uses of Politics. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1967
[1964].
Farley,
Reynolds, and Walter R. Allen
The Color Line and the Quality of Life in America. New York, Russell Sage
Foundation, 1987.
Farley,
Reynolds
"Residential
Segregation of Social and Economic Groups among Blacks, 1970-1980." In
Christopher Jencks and Paul E. Peterson, eds. The Urban Underclass.
Washington, DC: The Brookings Institution, 1991.
Gamson,
William A.
Talking Politics. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1992.
Gerth,
H. H., and C. Wright Mills, eds.
From Max Weber: Essays in Sociology. New York: Oxford University Press,
1946.
Guest,
Avery M., and James J. Zuiches
"Another look at Residential Turnover in Urban Neighborhoods: A Note on
'Racial Change in a Stable Community' by Harvey Molotch." American
Journal of Sociology 77 (1971):457-67.
Hirsch,
Arnold
"Race and Housing: Violence and Communal Protest in Chicago,
1940-1960." In Melvin G. Holli and Peter d'A. Jones, eds. The Ethnic
Frontier. Grand Rapids, MI: Eerdmans Publishing, 1977.
Hochschild,
Jennifer L.
What's Fair? Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1981.
Hunter,
Albert
Symbolic Communities. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1974.
Iyengar,
Shanto
Is Anyone Responsible?: How Television Frames Political Issues. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press, 1991.
Kirschenman,
Joleen, and Kathyryn M. Neckerman
" 'We'd Love to Hire Them, But...': The Meaning of Race for
Employers." In Christopher Jencks and Paul E. Peterson, eds. The Urban
Underclass. Washington, DC: The Brookings Institution, 1991.
Kitagawa,
Evelyn M., and Karl E. Taeuber, eds.
Local Community Fact Book Chicago Metropolitan Area 1960. Chicago
Community Inventory, University of Chicago, 1963.
Kleppner,
Paul
Chicago Divided: The Making of a Black Mayor. DeKalb, IL: Northern
Illinois University Press, 1985.
Lane,
Robert
Political Ideology: Why the American Common Man Believes What He Does.
New York: The Free Press of Glenco, 1962.
Lasswell,
Harold D.
Politics: Who Gets What, When, How. New York: Peter Smith, 1950 [1936].
Lasswell,
Harold D.
"The Language of Power." In Harold D. Lasswell, Nathan Leites, and
Associates Language of Politics. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press, 1965
[1949].
Manley
Jr., Theodoric
By the Color of Their Skins: Sociopolitical Transformations in a Chicago
Neighborhood. Dissertation, Department of Sociology, University of Chicago,
June 1986.
McClosky,
Herbert, and John Zaller
The American Ethos. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1984.
Massey,
Douglas S., and Nancy A. Denton
"The Dimensions of Residential Segregation." Social Forces 67
(1988):281-315.
Massey,
Douglas S., and Nancy A. Denton
American Apartheid: Segregation and the Making of the Underclass.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1993.
Massey,
Douglas S.
"The Residential Segregation of Blacks, Hispanics, and Asians,
1970-1990." Unpublished. Population Research Center, The University of
Chicago, 1993.
Molotch,
Harvey
"Racial Change in a Stable Community." American Journal of
Sociology 75 (1969):226-38.
Molotch,
Harvey
"Reply to Guest and Zuiches: Another Look at Residential Turnover in Urban
Neighborhoods." American Journal of Sociology 77 (1971):468-71.
Molotch,
Harvey Luskin
Managed Integration. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1972.
Rieder,
Jonathan
Canarsie: The Jews and Italians of Brooklyn Against Liberalism.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1985.
Sanders,
Arthur
Making Sense of Politics. Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press, 1990.
Schank,
Roger C.
Tell Me a Story. New York: Macmillan, 1990.
Schelling,
Thomas C.
Micromotives and Macrobehavior. New York: Norton, 1978.
Schuman,
Howard, Charlotte Steeh, and Lawrence Bobo Racial Attitudes in America.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1988 [1985].
Sigel,
Roberta S.
"Conclusion: Adult Political Socialization, A Lifelong Process." In
Roberta S. Sigel, ed. Political Learning in Adulthood. Chicago: The
University of Chicago Press, 1989.
Sklare,
Marshall
Observing America's Jews. Hanover: Brandeis University Press, 1993.
Sklare,
Marshall, and Joseph Greenblum
Jewish Identity on the Suburban Frontier. Chicago: The University of
Chicago Press, 1979 [1967].
Smith,
Tom W.
"Changing Racial Labels: From 'Colored' to 'Negro' to 'Black' to
'African-American'." Public Opinion Quarterly 56 (1992):496-514.
Soukhanov,
Anne H., ed.
Webster's II New Riverside University Dictionary. Boston: Riverside
Publishing, 1984.
Spear,
Allan H.
Black Chicago: The Making of a Negro Ghetto, 1890-1920. Chicago: The
University of Chicago Press, 1967.
Symposium
in American Political Science Review 85 (1991):1341-80.
James
H. Kuklinski, Robert C. Luskin, and John Bolland
"Where is the Schema?
Going Beyond the 'S' word in Political Psychology." pp. 1341-56.
Pamela
Johnston Conover and Stanley Feldman, Milton Lodge and Kathleen M. McGraw, and Arthur H. Miller
"Where
is the Schema? Critiques." pp. 1357-80.
Taub,
Richard P., D. Garth Taylor, and Jan D. Dunham Paths of Neighborhood Change: Race
and Crime in Urban America. Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 1984.
Tidwell,
Billy J., ed.
The State of Black America 1992. New York: The National Urban League,
Inc., 1992.
U.S.
Bureau of the Census
of Population and Housing, 1990 Summary Tape File 3A on CD-ROM.
U.S.
Bureau of the Census
Statistical Abstract of the United States: 1992. (112th edition)
Washington, DC, 1992.
Wilson,
William Julius
The Truly Disadvantaged. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press,
(1987).
Young, Michael L. Dictionary of Polling: The Language of Contemporary Opinion Research. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992.
ENDNOTES
[1]Webster's
II New Riverside University Dictionary,
p. 736, 1 and 2.
[8]Published
version of a speech, "Politics as a Vocation" at Munich University
by Max Weber in 1918. In Gerth
and Mills (1946), p. 78.
[21]Massey
(1993), table 1. Source for 1990: Roderick J. Harrison and Daniel H.
Weinberg, "Racial and Ethnic Segregation in 1990," Paper presented
at the Annual Meetings of the Population Association of America, Denver
1992.
[24]Farley
and Allen (1987), p. 146. While
Russians have not been as segregated as Blacks, like Blacks, Russians have
experienced more discriminatory barriers in housing markets and higher
levels of segregation than have other ethnic groups.
Russians were the most segregated European ethnic group in the nation
in 1980. In 8 of the 16
metropolitan areas the Russian-English segregation index was 60 or higher,
and in 2 cities, Baltimore and St. Louis, the Russian-English indexes were
73 and 75 respectively. Farley
and Allen (1980), Table 5.9.
[36]Farley
(1991), Table 1. Source: Robert
Wilger and Reynolds Farley, "Black-White Residential Segregation:
Recent Trends." University of Michigan, Population Studies Center,
1989.
[38]Massey
(1993), tables 2 and 3. Source for 1990: Roderick J. Harrison and Daniel H.
Weinberg, "Racial and Ethnic Segregation in 1990," Paper presented
at the Annual Meetings of the Population Association of America, Denver
1992. The European ethnic group in the Chicago Metropolitan Area
most segregated from the English in 1980 was the Russians, and the
Russian-English segregation index was 64.
The Black-English segregation index was 80 in the Chicago
Metropolitan Area in 1980. Farley
and Allen (1989), Table 5.9.
[41]Burgess
(1967), p. 7. The ecological dimension of community refers to the selective
spatial distributions of populations and functions and to interaction
mediated through the spatial and physical environment.
The normative dimension of community includes, first, normative
social interaction and resulting social structure, and, second the cultural
and symbolic elements of community - the shared collective representations
and moral sentiments. The
ability to exchange meaning through a shared set of symbols has long been
recognized as an integral part of a community.
Hunter (1974), pp. 4, 19, 67.
[43]The
11 South Shore neighborhoods (according to Molotch) are Parkside, the
Jackson Park Highlands, O'Keeffe, Bryn-Mawr West, Bryn-Mawr East, South End
West, South End East, Windsor Park, South Shore Drive, and also Bradwell and
Cheltenham whose residents did not as clearly identify themselves with other
South Shore residents due to social differences (Russian
"ethnicity"), physical features, such as major thoroughfares which
naturally divide areas, and housing structure type and social class
variation. Molotch (1972),
Chapter 3.
[44]Molotch
(1972), p. 44; Molotch (1969). At
the time of Molotch's study, South Shore and Rogers Park were two community
areas that were comparable on numerous population and housing
characteristics, their lakefront locations, and their distances of about 10
miles from downtown Chicago on a north-south axis.
[51]A
large number of persons of Russian stock resided in South Shore during the
decades before racial succession. Foreign
stock includes foreign-born persons and persons of foreign or mixed
parentage. In 1930, the foreign
stock population in South Shore (Fact Book boundaries) was 52.2 percent of
the total population, 78,755. By
foreign stock, the total population in South Shore in 1930 was 9.3 percent
German, 8.5 percent Irish, 7.8 percent from the United Kingdom, 4.4 percent
Canadian, and 4.1 percent from the Soviet Union.
In 1960 the largest foreign stock group was the Soviet Union,
comprising 7.0 percent of the total population.
Local Community Fact Book Chicago Metropolitan Area 1960, p.
101.
[57]Lodge
and McGraw, Conover and Feldman, and Miller, all in "Where is the
Schema" Symposium in American Political Science Review 85
(1991):1341-80.
[59]Conover
and Feldman in "Where is the Schema" symposium in American
Political Science Review 85 (1991):1341-80.
"Where Is the Schema? Critiques?" p. 1365.
[63]Iyengar
(1991), p. 8. The global view
posits the existence of overarching, higher-order constructs (such as
liberal or conservative ideology, political party affiliation, subjective
utility, self-interest, and socioeconomic and cultural values) which result
in distinct opinion profiles for people who can be classified according to
these constructs. The domain-specific approaches hypothesize that opinions are
based on narrower and more focused considerations relevant to particular
issues (pp. 7,9).
[65]Iyengar
(1991), p. 128. Iyengar
advances this theory in a study of the contextual effects of television news
frames on political opinionation. The
hypothesis is that the manner in which a news story is framed is important
because different types of news frames elicit different types of attribution
of political responsibility, which are in turn important through their role
in political opinionation. Iyengar (1991), p. 14.
[68]For
the Black residents of South Shore these concerns and opinions of outsiders
have to do with an unwillingness to understand the intimate knowledge of
social life in the South Shore. Manley (1986), pp.30-31.
[69]The
Jackson Park Highlands is an area of large and expensive homes which has
long been the most clearly delineated neighborhood in South Shore and the
area most highly differentiated from surrounding areas in terms of social
and physical circumstances. "It
[Jackson Park] is the highest status residential area of South Shore,
consisting of large, well-maintained brick and stone homes on elaborate
grounds. Other people think of
Highlands residents as uniformly "rich"-sometimes speaking of
residents as millionaires." Molotch (1972), p. 51.